Manual Dexterity Activities For Adults

Posted on by
Manual Dexterity Activities For Adults Average ratng: 4,7/5 4275 votes
Published online 2015 Feb 17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117598

A dexterity exercise like this one is a fun way to play and get those muscles of the hand moving and strengthened in order to improve endurance and positioning. Looking for more fun ways to practice finger dexterity? These are some fun games and activities you may want to try: Beach Play Dough Itsy Bitsy Spider and other finger play games. Amharic orthodox bible pdf.

PMID: 25689161

An added bonus is tons of finger dexterity practice along the way. Fine Motor Activities Using Dice from Still Playing School – Kids just love playing and learning with dice! These six ideas for using dice to work on children’s hand and finger dexterity are so simple and fun. Maintaining hand dexterity for as long as possible enables independence and the easy management of daily tasks in senior years. While it can be harder to maintain manual dexterity and fine motor skills as we age, there are easy daily exercises and activities that can keep your fingers flexible and effective.

Fine motor skills, speed & dexterity, and fine motor activities for Boys & Girls of All Ages. Work on speed and dexterity with balancing games, stacking games, scavenger hunt games, and an electronic balance challenge. Activities for Manual Dexterity You can improve your manual dexterity by performing activities that require repetition, focus, hand-eye coordination and a steady hand. Activities such as learning. Manual Dexterity. Manual dexterity involves the development of hand-eye coordination and very fine motor control. Imagine the level of care and precision required in certain activities, hobbies and professions that involve small, meticulous actions with your hands, such as sewing, painting and playing a musical instrument. Manual dexterity is the ability of the hands and fingers to make coordinated movements. Strong fine motor skills, such as used with writing, knitting, sewing, and other activities that involve the hands and fingers, rely on this dexterity. In young children, it is developed normally through routine activities that also require hand-eye.

Andrea Macaluso, Academic Editor
This article has been cited by other articles in PMC.

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
S1 Fig: Scatterplot of age and grip strength for the non-dominant hand. (TIFF)
GUID: 82A076DA-D300-464E-9D58-3B33DE96A63F

Abstract

In the scientific literature, there is much evidence of a relationship between age and dexterity, where increased age is related to slower, less nimble and less smooth, less coordinated and less controlled performances. While some suggest that the relationship is a direct consequence of reduced muscle strength associated to increased age, there is a lack of research that has systematically investigated the relationships between age, strength and hand dexterity. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine the associations between age, grip strength and dexterity. 107 adults (range 18-93 years) completed a series of hand dexterity tasks (i.e. steadiness, line tracking, aiming, and tapping) and a test of maximal grip strength. We performed three phases of analyses. Firstly, we evaluated the simple relationships between pairs of variables; replicating the existing literature; and found significant relationships of increased age and reduced strength; increased age and reduced dexterity, and; reduced strength and reduced dexterity. Secondly, we used standard Multiple Regression (MR) models to determine which of the age and strength factors accounted for the greater variance in dexterity. The results showed that both age and strength made significant contributions to the data variance, but that age explained more of the variance in steadiness and line tracking dexterity, whereas strength explained more of the variance in aiming and tapping dexterity. In a third phase of analysis, we used MR analyses to show an interaction between age and strength on steadiness hand dexterity. Simple Slopes post-hoc analyses showed that the interaction was explained by the middle to older aged adults showing a relationship between reduced strength and reduced hand steadiness, whereas younger aged adults showed no relationship between strength and steadiness hand dexterity. The results are discussed in terms of how age and grip strength predict different types of hand dexterity in adults.

Introduction

The hand is the most active and interactive part of the upper extremity. Hand dexterity is a term used to explain a range of different hand abilities and performances. These include reaction time; hand preference; wrist flexion speed; finger tapping speed; aiming; hand stability and arm stability (e.g., []). From these, four main factors are considered as the most characteristic and reliable for the evaluation of hand dexterity. These include: (i) steadiness; (ii) tracking; (iii) aiming (where typically the participant points to a target object), and (iv) tapping (where the participant taps as fast as possible for a set time period) ([]; []; [3]; []; []).

The relationship between increased age and reduced hand dexterity has been widely reported in both the clinical and scientific literature. For example, [] presented the first kinematic assessment that compared the reach-to-grasp movements for gender-matched groups of older aged (60–71 years, n = 12) and younger aged (18–25 years, n = 12) adults. Participants reached to grasp either a small cylinder using a precision grip or a large cylinder using a whole hand prehension. The actions performed by the two groups were similarly coordinated with similar times to peak for wrist velocity and acceleration from movement initiation (i.e. the transport component), and showed no differences in the size of the grip apertures used (i.e. object manipulation). However, the older aged participants made significantly slower movements than the younger aged adults, replicating previous findings (e.g., []; []; []; []; []). Although movement speed can be encapsulated within the term of hand dexterity, it is worth noting that slower movements with increased age may not necessarily correspond to a reduced performance for the other dexterity factors (e.g., aiming, stability etc.). This point will be investigated in the present study.

Despite numerous studies demonstrating a significant relationship between increased age and reduced hand dexterity, few studies have attempted to investigate the causes of the relationship. Instead, a common explanation is provided in the discussion of these papers stating that the relationship between increased age and reduced hand dexterity is likely caused by a decline in musculoskeletal strength and mass (see for example []; []; []; []; [16]; []; [18]). Much support can be found for these claims within related literature, with major reduction in muscle mass ranging from 20% to 45% in aging skeletal muscle (described as “sarcopenia of old age”; [], p477) (see also []; []; []; []; []; []). More precise investigations of hand strength have also demonstrated diminished strength with increased age (e.g., []; []; []; []; []), with studies reporting that diminished hand strength appears associated to decreasing general muscle mass reduction ([]; []; []; []). Furthermore, changes in muscle mass with increased age has been linked to changes in peripheral and central nerve conduction ([]; []; []), proprioception ([]) and changes in the human motor unit that relate to a degeneration of the nervous system ([]; []); all of which are likely to impact hand dexterity.

Given the overwhelming evidence for relationships between age and hand dexterity, and between age and grip strength, it is perhaps surprising that there are very few studies that have investigated the relationships between age, grip strength and hand dexterity together in one study. Instead, the few studies considering the three factors in a single study have investigated differences in these factors for different age groups of participants. For example, Marmon et al. [] tested three groups of adult participants (young 18–36, middle 40–60 and older ≥ 65 years aged adults) on measures of index finger abduction, precision pinch, and hand grip strength and measures of hand dexterity with the Grooved Pegboard test, the game Operation, a scissor task and a tracing task. The results showed significant differences between the young and older age groups for the measures of index finger abduction, precision pinch and handgrip strengths, and further for the measures of hand dexterity. A further analysis showed evidence of significant associations among similar tests (i.e. the strength measures; the steadiness measures and the four function measures) and between different measures. As a final multiple regression analysis showed that the time to complete two functional measures, the Grooved Pegboard and the game Operation were significantly predicted by index finger and grip strength or by pinch steadiness, index finger steadiness and grip strength respectively.

The aim of the current study was to extend these findings and investigate, using regression analyses, the relationships and interactions between age, grip strength and hand dexterity in adults. We hypothesised to replicate previous research by showing negative relations between age and strength, and age and hand dexterity, with increased age being related to reduced strength and hand dexterity. We also hypothesised a positive relationship between strength and dexterity (so that reduced strength was related to reduced hand dexterity). In a second phase of analyses, we then sought to extend the research findings by determining the variance of age and strength on the different components of dexterity (i.e. steadiness; tracking; aiming and tapping). In a final phase of analyses, we explored interactions in age and strength on hand dexterity.

Methods

Ethics Statement

All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study, and the study was approved by the School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Birmingham ethical committee in accordance with the ethical standards established by the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants

A total of 107 participants, aged 18–93 years were tested (60 female: mean 50 ± 21 years; range 18–86 years, and 57 male: mean 48 ± 18 years; range 20–93 years). All participants were recruited from the city of Birmingham (UK) following advertising the project at local community social groups and by giving short presentations about the project. Prior to giving an invitation to participate in the study, potential participants were pre-screened via an informal verbal discussion to determine eligibility. The inclusion criteria were that participants must be 18+ years of age, able to travel unaided to the University of Birmingham (i.e. by walking, driving or using public transport) and reported no muscular skeletal, arthritis or neurological problems. This information was collected using a purpose written self-report health questionnaire to assess for arthritis, and an informal verbal interview, all performed immediately prior to the testing sessions. These criteria were used to ensure that only participants in good ‘health’ were tested and that healthy age effects could be explored without being potentially confounded by ill health influencing the results. For clarity, good ‘health’ was classified as the absence of any diagnosed physical diseases, mental illnesses, recent surgeries or hospitalisation, feelings of illness, no reported sign of arthritis and with a high score (27/30) on the Mini Mental State Examination []. Any participants not meeting the criteria were not included in the study.

Apparatus and Design

For the main study, we assessed hand dominance (Edinburgh handedness inventory: []) and measured grip strength for both hands and dexterity for the dominant hand only. To measure grip strength we used a handgrip dynamometer (Takei Scientific Instruments, Japan) that has been shown to be an easy, fast and reliable method ([]). Participants were positioned sitting upright, with their elbows by their left and right sides, and flexed to right angles. A neutral wrist position held the dynamometer in their hand to be tested. The handle of the dynamometer was adjusted so that the base of the dynamometer was positioned with the rest on first metacarpal (heel of palm), and the handle rested on the middle of the four fingers prior to testing. The results were recorded as kilograms taken from the digital display of the dynamometer to the nearest 0.1 kg. The digital display of the dynamometer displayed the maximum strength within a trial and the value was reset to zero before each subsequent measurement.

Dexterity was measured using the ‘Vienna Test System: Motor Performance Series Workboard’ (VTS: MLS; Lafayette Instrument, Model 64030, Lafayette, IN [40]; developed by Schoppe [41]; and based on Fleishman’s factor-analytic examinations of fine motor abilities in arms: [42]; See Fig. 1). The test is a standardised diagnostic tool used to provide valid and reliable assessments of hand dexterity (see [43]; [44]; [45]). The VTS part of the test consists of a main computer with test interface management software and the MLS part consists of a peripheral panel containing a variety of different tests. Test-retest reliability has been reported for the MLS to range from. 60 to. 94, dependent on the dexterity measure used ([45]). From the MLS tests, we used measures of steadiness, line tracking, aiming and tapping (explained in the procedure section); thus measuring the different components most characteristic for the evaluation of hand dexterity performance ([]; []; [3]; []; []). The MLS was placed on a wooden testing table, and participants sat on a stable wooden chair with back support in a normal sitting posture with slightly bent forearms. Participants were positioned at the mid-point of the table, perpendicular to the MLS and throughout the tests were instructed not to make contact with the wooden table or MLS (i.e. so that no additional postural support was achieved).

A schematic diagram of the motor performance series work panel.

Procedure

Participants took part in one session that lasted approximately ninety minutes. At the beginning of the session, after providing informed consent, participants were given the set of questionnaires that were used for the inclusion criteria. If a participant was not compatible with the inclusion criteria, they were not included in the study. Following the questionnaires, and confirmation of their inclusion, participants performed the grip dynamometry assessment. The participant squeezed the dynamometer with maximum effort for at least 5 seconds. Handgrip strength was tested three times with a minimum of 60 seconds rest was provided for recovery between each attempt. Maximum grip strength was recorded as the highest value of three trials. Note that the maximum value was used rather than the mean of three trials allowing for a measure that was independent of fatigue, which may have been possible in some of the participants.

Next participants completed the series of fine motor tests using the VTS: MLS. Before commencement of the selected MLS tests, each test was verbally explained and demonstrated to the participant with a brief period provided for the participant to familiarise themselves with each test. The VTS management software controlled the order of the MLS tests and recorded all measurements. The same order of tests was used for all participants (as the software was unable to randomise the order of the tests). At the end of each test sequence, a break was provided.

In the fixed sequence of tests, the first MLS test was the steadiness test. This involved the participant to vertically insert a pen stylus (2mm diameter) into a pre-assigned hole (5.8mm diameter) and then hold the stylus as still as possible for 32 seconds without touching the sides or bottom of the hole. If the participant touched the sides they simply had to correct and position the stylus in the centre of the hole and continue until the end of the 32 seconds period. Measurements recorded by the software were the number of times the stylus came into contact with the sides or the bottom of the hole and the total duration of the contact time. The second test was line tracking. This involved the insertion of the pen stylus into a grooved track and tracing the pen stylus through the track without touching the sides or the bottom. The test was undertaken at a self-selected pace. If the sides were touched, participants had to correct the position of the pen stylus and continue along the grooved track until the end. Measurements recorded were the number of times the stylus touched the side or bottom, the total duration of any touches and the total duration of the tracking. The third test was the aiming test. This involved the participant touching 20 brass disks (5mm diameter) that were arranged in a row with the pen stylus. The distance between each disk was 4mm. The participant was instructed to tap one circle in the row after the other using the pen stylus, moving from right to the left across the disks. Measurements recorded were the number of correct taps, the number of incorrect taps, the total duration of any incorrect tap and the total duration to complete. The final task was tapping. This involved the participant tapping a square plate (40 mm2) with the pen stylus as many times as within 32 seconds. The measurement recorded was the total number of taps. The VTS software recorded all measurements. Each dependent variable consisted of a composite performance score calculated using the Fleishman-factor of weighted variables from each measure (e.g., a composite of the number of touch errors and the total duration of any touches for line tracking etc.) (See [3]; [45]; []).

Data Analyses

The data analysis was undertaken in three phases. In the first phase, we report the simple relationships between two of the variables using simple regression analysis; between age and strength; age and each dependent measure of the MLS; and strength and each dependent measure of the MLS. The aims of these first analyses were to replicate the existing findings in the literature. In the second phase of analysis, the three variables were compared together using standard multiple regression. In a third phase, we modified the variables to allow analyses of interactions. The independent variables of age and grip strength were mean centred to provide numerical stability (see recommendations by Cohen and Cohen [47]; and Aitken and West [48]). Prior to the standard Multiple Regressions (MR) preliminary analyses were conducted to ensure no violation of the assumptions of normality; linearity; multicollinearity and homoscedasticity occurred, using a criterion of p < 0.001 for Mahalanobis distance. No outliers among the cases were found. Then a standard MR was used to analyse the predictive variance between the independent variables of age and strength, and each dependent dexterity measure. Where both independent variables had a significant contribution to the dependent variable, a further standard MR analysis was undertaken using the mean centred data and the mean centred cross product of age and strength to test for an interaction (see [47]). In addition, post hoc Simple Slopes Technique analyses were used to further investigate the interaction (see [47]; [48]). This was done to determine specific values of (centred) age, that were one standard deviation below the sample mean, the mean, and one standard deviation above the mean; in this instance to represent low aged, mean aged and high aged adults, respectively, and the same to represent low strength, mean strength and high strength adults, respectively. These values were then entered into the regression equation Y = (b1+b3Z)X + (b2Z+b0) to calculate the data to plot the simple slopes for analysis, where X = Strength and Z = Age. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v21 (SPSS, Chicago, Illinois, USA), except the Simple Slopes Technique that was performed in Matlab (Mathworks, Massachusetts, USA) using a bespoke script.

Results

Phase 1: Simple Linear Regressions

Analyses of the simple relationships between variables showed a significant negative relationship between age and grip strength (r = -0.42, p<0.001; see Fig. 2a; see also S1 Fig.). As hypothesised, this showed that increased age was related to decreased strength, with the linear best fit showing a decline of 0.25 kg for each year of age. There were also significant simple relationships between age and each dependent measure of the MLS (see Fig. 2b) (steadiness: r = 0.56, p<0.001; line tracking: r = 0.61, p<0.001; aiming: r = -0.46, p<0.001, and: tapping: r = 0.51, p<0.001) and between strength and each dependent measure of the MLS (see Fig. 2c) (steadiness: r = -0.42, p<0.001; line tracking: r = -0.40, p<0.001; aiming: r = 0.57, p<0.001, and: tapping: r = 0.62, p<0.001).

Nine scatterplots to illustrate the relationships between: (A) age and grip strength; (B) the dependent measures of hand dexterity and age; and (C) the dependent measures of hand dexterity and strength.

Also shown on each plot is the best-fit simple regression line and the linear regression equation (***p<0.001, n = 107). NB: Dependent measures are represented in the following order: (i) steadiness; (ii) tracking; (iii) aiming; (iv) tapping; with the direction of a positive/negative performance score indicated.

Phase 2: Multiple Regression (MR) Analysis

The results of the MR analyses are presented in Tables Tables11 and and2.2. Table 1 shows the results of the standard MR analyses. This showed that both variables of age and strength significantly predicted the variance of each hand dexterity dependent variable. However, the results showed that age predicted more variance than strength for the hand dexterity measures of steadiness and line tracking, whereas strength predicted more variance than age for the hand dexterity measures of aiming and tapping. Table 2 shows the results of the MR analyses in which the product of the age and strength variables only showed a significant interactions effect for age and strength on the hand dexterity dependent variable of steadiness. Fig. 3 presents these interaction effects between age and strength for each of the hand dexterity dependent variables in four separate MR plane fit plots (the data points are not plotted).

Table 1

The results of the multiple regression analysis for age and strength on each movement dexterity task.
VariablesParameter BSEStandardised βModel R2sr2 (unique)
Steadiness
Intercept5.120.58
Age0.17 ***0.03.460.18
Strength- 0.14 **0.05-.230.35 ***0.04
Line Tracking
Intercept14.800.78
Age0.28 ***0.04.530.23
Strength- 0.15 **0.7-.180.40 ***0.03
Aiming
Intercept35.860.37
Age- 0.06 ***0.02-.260.06
Strength0.19 ***0.03.460.39 ***0.18
Tapping
Intercept63.280.62
Age- 0.13 ***0.03-.300.08
Strength0.35 ***0.06.490.46 ***0.20

**p < 0.01;

Table 2

The results of the multiple regression interaction analyses (age, strength and age x strength) for each dexterity task.
Independent VariablesParameter BSEStandardised βModel R2sr2 (unique)
Steadiness
Intercept4.390.63
Age0.14 ***0.03.380.10
Strength-0.12 *0.05-.200.03
Age X Strength-0.01 *0.00-.220.35 ***0.04
Line Tracking
Intercept14.800.87
Age0.25 ***0.05.490.18
Strength-0.14 *0.07-.170.02
Age X Strength-0.010.00-.110.40 ***0.00
Aiming
Intercept35.860.42
Age-0.07 **0.02-.270.05
Strength0.19 ***0.03.470.18
Age X Strength0.000.00-.030.39 ***0.00
Tapping
Intercept63.250.69
Age-0.12 **0.04-.280.06
Strength0.34 ***0.06.490.19
Age X Strength0.000.00.060.46 ***0.00

**p < 0.01;

Three-dimensional plot of the best-fit multiple regression plane relating age and strength for each dependent measures of hand dexterity (i: steadiness; ii: line tracking; iii: aiming; iv: tapping).

Phase 3: Simple Slopes Post-Hoc Analyses

The interaction effects were analysed using Simple Slopes post-hoc analyses. For the interaction effect for steadiness dexterity, the high aged (68.8 years) and mean aged (48.8 years) adults showed a significant positive relationship between strength and steadiness, whereas lower aged (28.8 years) adults showed no relation between strength and steadiness. No other interaction analyses were performed (see Fig. 4).

Four plots illustrating the simple slope post hoc interaction analyses: (i) strength and steadiness; (ii) strength and line tracking; (iii) strength and aiming; (iv) strength and tapping.

Data plotted are mean centred age ± 1 SD, where minus one standard deviation represents younger adults (Low age) and plus one standard deviation represents older adults (High age). Note that the direction of positive performance score is indicated.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to examine the relation between age, grip strength and hand dexterity. The motor tests were chosen to reflect those factors most characteristic and reliable for the evaluation of hand dexterity. Here, we firstly replicated previous literature showing significant simple relationships between age and strength, age and hand dexterity, and strength and hand dexterity. Increased age was significantly associated with decreased strength and decreased hand dexterity, and decreased strength was significantly associated with decreased hand dexterity. MR analyses showed that performance on steadiness and line tracking dexterity tests were better explained by age than strength, but aiming and tapping dexterity were better explained by strength than age. Analyses of the interaction between age and strength for hand dexterity showed a significant interaction for steadiness hand dexterity. Post-hoc analyses of the steadiness interaction highlight that the older and mean aged adults showed reduced hand steadiness with reduced strength, but that there was no effect for younger adults.

The finding of a simple relationship between age and grip strength replicated previous studies in the literature (e.g., []; []; []; []; []; []; []; []; []; [53]; []). Furthermore, the finding that grip strength reduced by 0.25 kg or 1.4% per year was roughly consistent to previous findings showing that grip strength declines annually between 1%–1.5% ([]; []). We know that grip strength is linked to reductions in muscle mass ([]; []; []), and that age associated reductions in muscle mass are also consistently linked to changes in the properties of muscle activation and muscle recruitment that likely have an impact on hand dexterity. For example, notable age-related changes in muscle properties are: a slowed rate of muscle contraction ([]; []); a slowed neural conduction velocity ([]; []); and increased muscle antagonist coactivation that’s necessary to stabilise a joint during a movement ([]; []), but has the by-product of also restricting joint movement. The simple relations between age and the hand dexterity measures of steadiness, line-tracking, aiming and tapping also replicated existing literature, showing a reduced performance with increased age ([]; []; []; []; []; []). For example, Marmon et al. [], recently evaluated finger dexterity using the Grooved Pegboard test along with measures of strength, steadiness and other hand functions. For all of their measures, they observed that increased age was associated with reduced performance and that progressive declines in strength (i.e. grip, pinch and index finger abduction) are observed with increased age. In the final simple relation that we investigated, we showed that all hand dexterity tasks significantly declined with decreased strength. This indicates that adults with weaker grip typically had worse movement dexterity than stronger adults.

While our study is not the first study to show associations between age and hand dexterity or strength and hand dexterity (as discussed above), this is the first study that provides evidence of associations between the three factors of age, strength and hand dexterity using standard MR analyses. These analyses showed that age and strength significantly moderated hand dexterity, with the two variables explaining between 35% and 46% of the different hand dexterity tasks variance. This finding is consistent with the findings from the simple two variable correlation analyses. As adults increased in age, their strength and hand dexterity declined. Interestingly, these results showed two main findings. This was that hand dexterity performance on steadiness and line tracking were better explained by age than strength, but aiming and tapping dexterity were better explained by strength than age. We discuss each of these findings in turn.

The hand dexterity tasks of steadiness and line tracking both rely on a stable control of the arm and hand, and eye-hand coordination with visual guidance. Strength had little predictive value to the variance of steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity, suggesting that other factors may have influenced the result. The other factors could have included vision, or cognitive capacity for online visual guidance; though we excluded participants with impaired visual acuity, vision or cognitive capacity; it is clear that all these factors deteriorate with age ([63]). Also, Marmon et al. [], who also reported reduced steadiness hand dexterity with older compared to younger aged adults, found the effect irrespective of whether the task was performed with or without visual feedback, suggesting that factors of reduced visual guidance with increased age may be less relevant. An alternative explanation for the poor steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity with increased age could be a consequence of the control of hand muscle force, known to fluctuate more in older adults ([]; []; []; []; []; []). These moderations in muscle control are thought to involve changes in average motor unit force output ([]; []), antagonist coactivation ([]; []; []), and motor unit discharge variability ([]; []), and are unrelated to muscle strength per se. A similar explanation could be that tremor-related movements caused by irregular fluctuations in muscle and limb displacement ([]; []; []) reduced steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity performance. We propose that future studies should investigate the role of muscle control and tremor on steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity in ageing.

The results of the MR analyses showed that the variance of aiming and tapping hand dexterity tasks was more explained by grip strength than age. This suggests that dexterous actions that rely on the fast and precise coordinated movement control of the hand, wrist, elbow and shoulder, such as that of aiming and tapping, appear directly associated to hand grip strength. Although this is the first time that this point is made, it is well known that the successful coordination of muscles during movement execution depends on well-regulated muscular forces, sensory information and body scheme ([76]; [77]; [78]). The presence of fluctuations in muscle force during a movement influences the capacity to produce the intended movement correctly, causing compensations within the movement. In aiming and tapping, the thumb and index finger must pinch the stylus object with a minimum force that must be greater than the friction force required to prevent object slipping when lifting the object ([]; []). The performance of the swift aiming and tapping movements then requires large (and sequenced) muscle activations with rapid accelerations of movement to respond to the task. At the same time, the thumb and index finger pinch of the stylus must coordinate force with the movement accelerations and the impact with the target. Therefore, reduced strength will lead to increases in the variability of force capability, causing increases in variability to the movement trajectory and the accuracy of the final position ([]; []; []; []; []). These effects will be compounded in repeat performances of a task (e.g., the tapping task) in that fluctuations in muscle force during a voluntary contraction will cause an increase in the variance of movement kinematics from trial to trial ([]; [85]; []; []; [88]; []; [])

The suggestion in our data here is that having a higher level of grip strength will lead to a higher aiming and tapping ability. Currently, this view is speculative, but we propose that increased grip strength will be associated with fewer fluctuations in muscle force during movement execution, allowing for faster, more fluent, less variable and more stable successful successive movements. Support for our speculation is present in existing literature where increases in muscle strength, after just a few weeks of strength training, have been associated with decreases in force fluctuations during isometric and anisometric contractions in a number of muscles (e.g. first dorsal interosseus muscle and knee extensor muscles) ([]; []; []; [94]) (for a review of mechanisms that contribute to differences in motor performance between young and old adults, see []). Additionally, individuals who regularly practice rapid, coordinated, goal directed movements, such as expert musicians, appear not to display the same age related decrements of dexterity that are typical of age matched non-musical adults ([]; []). Such an observation suggests that regular exercise is possibly sufficient to maintain rapid, coordinated, goal directed dexterous actions (such as tapping and aiming). If correct, strength exercises in adults may be a simple preventative measure against hand dexterity decline, independent of age. We propose that future research should test where moderations in strength cause changes in rapid, coordinated, goal directed movements such as tapping and aiming hand dexterity.

A final novel finding of this paper was that age and strength variables interacted with the hand dexterity measure of steadiness. The post hoc analyses indicated that the mean aged and older aged adults showed a relationship where reduced strength was associated to reduced steadiness hand dexterity, but the younger aged adults showed no relationship between strength and steadiness hand dexterity. This interaction result was interesting, as it appeared to suggest that steadiness hand dexterity was moderated by strength, only for older adults. This finding needs further investigation. It is unclear whether strength training interventions would be beneficial to steadiness hand dexterity in the same way as the exercises proposed to improve aiming and tapping hand dexterity, discussed above. It may be that other factors such as muscle control may co-vary with grip strength, explaining the apparent strength effect here. Beyond strength, changes in touch sensation may be considered as a plausible factor to explain such changes; however, in light of a recent research study [] it would appear that any age-related reductions in sensation and motor function cannot easily be inferred from each other, and as such do not offer a simple explanation.

Our study presents some novel findings and some factors that we propose future studies should include and consider. The finding that steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity was moderated more by age than strength suggests that another factor must contribute to the moderation in performance. In the discussion above, we suggested that muscle control and / or tremor may have been a causal factor, and we advocate the measure of these factors in future studies. Additionally, future studies should consider the effects of impaired visual acuity upon aspects of hand dexterity performance such as steadiness; here we had no in-depth assessment of visual acuity but simply controlled vision with a self-report of corrected vision and an informal discussion.

Minecraft 1.9 download pc. Download the new version of Minecraft 1.9 1.9 1.9.2 1.9.3 1.9.4 which was removed many bugs and added new tricks and features that mi now and look at what awaits us: new blocks, items, battle with the Ender dragon, changes to mechanics, etc. And the first thing we will see going into the game is the cell with the shield icon which is the slot for our second hand which can put any objects.

In this study, we also showed that aiming and tapping hand dexterity was moderated more by strength than age, and we proposed that future studies should investigate the effect of exercise on these types of action performance. Furthermore, our study could have included control for levels of physical activity or specific dexterity skills (i.e. musicians, jugglers, etc…), and we propose that future studies manipulate or control these factors. In this study, we only assess handgrip strength but have no measure of wrist, elbow or shoulder strength or power; arms movements during the experimental measures were free to reflect natural unsupported functional performance as such handgrip strength may not allow us to fully account for different task strategies of whole arm function that may be interesting for future studies. A final proposal for future studies is the investigation of hand dominance or differences in participant gender in these effects. We know that the dominant hand is usually stronger than the non-dominant hand, and also that males are stronger than females. However, it is currently unclear how these factors interact with age. We might expect that strength differences between the dominant and non-dominant hand show parallel effects in the age and hand dexterity relationship. However, it is unclear whether the same parallel effect will be present for males compared to females.

In conclusion, the present research showed that age and grip strength were all statistically significant predictors of hand dexterity, and that aiming and tapping hand dexterity (involving rapid, coordinated, goal directed actions) appeared to be moderated by the factor of grip strength more than age. This suggests that physical activity may improve particular types of hand dexterity. The research also showed that steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity performance was better explained by age than strength, and an interaction between age and strength on steadiness hand dexterity showed that mean to older adults were particularly influenced by strength. These latter findings need further investigation, with aim to determine what other factors than strength appear to moderate steadiness and line tracking hand dexterity. Future research into hand function should be complemented by other neurophysiological recordings that can provide insight into the mechanisms responsible for age-related differences in muscular control.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig

Scatterplot of age and grip strength for the non-dominant hand.

(TIFF)

Acknowledgments

We are indebted to the volunteers that took part in the study and to the University of Birmingham and the University of Worcester who co-funded the research project. A special thank you from Jason Martin to Maria Bither for her love and support. The authors have nothing further to disclose.

Funding Statement

Funding for the individuals involved in the current study was received from the University of Birmingham and the University of Worcester. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

1. Barnsley RH, Rabinovich S (1970) Handedness: Proficiency versus stated preference. Perceptual and Motor Skills30: 343–362. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2. Annett M (1976) A coordination of hand preference and skill replicated. British Journal of Psychology67: 587–592. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3. Fleishman EA, Ellison GD (1962) A factor analysis of fine manipulative performance. Journal of Applied Psychology46: S96–105. [Google Scholar]
4. Provins KA, Cunliffe P (1972) The reliability of some motor performance tests of handedness. Neuropsychologia10: 199–206. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
5. Rigal RA (1992) Which handedness: preference or Performance?Perceptual and Motor Skills75: 851–866. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6. Bennett KMB, Castiello U (1994) Reach to grasp: Changes with age. Journal of Gerontology49B(1): 1–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
7. Amrhein PC, Stelmach GE, Goggin NL (1991) Age differences in the maintenance and restructuring of movement preparation. Psychology of Aging6: 451–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
8. Carnahan H, Vandervoort AA, Swanson LR (1998) The influence of aging and target motion on the control of Prehension. Experimental Aging Research24(3): 289–306. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
9. Cerella J (1985) Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin98: 67–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10. Smith CD, Umberger GH, Manning EL, Slevin JT, Wekstein DR, et al. (1999) Critical decline in fine motor hand movements in human aging. American Academy of Neurology53(7): 1458–1461. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
11. Wishart LR, Lee TD, Murdoch JE, Hodges NJ (2000) Effects of Aging on Automatic and Effortful Processes in Bimanual Coordination. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences55B(2): 85–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
12. Brown M, Sinacore DR, Host HH (1995) The relationship of strength to function in the older adult. Journals of Gerontology: Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences50: 55–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
13. Carmeli E, Patish H, Coleman R (2003) The Aging Hand. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences58(2): 146–152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
14. Enoka RM, Christou EA, Hunter SK, Kornatz KW, Semmler JG, et al. (2003) Mechanisms that contribute to differences in motor performance between young and old adults. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology13(1): 1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
15. Grabiner MD, Enoka RM (1995) Changes in movement capabilities with aging. Exercise & Sport Science Review23: 65–104. 10.1016/j.fsigen.2014.12.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
16. Harridge SDR, Young A (1998) Skeletal muscle In: Pathy MSJ, editor. Principles and practice of geriatric medicinevolume II (3rd edition). London: John Wiley & Sons; pp. 898–905. [Google Scholar]
17. Marmon AR, Pascoe MA, Schwartz RS, Enoka RM (2011) Associations among Strength, Steadiness, and Hand Function across the Adult Life Span. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise43(4): 560–567. 10.1016/j.bbalip.2015.01.004 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
18. Spirduso WW (1995) Physical dimensions of aging. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. [Google Scholar]
19. Carmeli E, Coleman R, Reznick AZ (2002) The biochemistry of gaining muscle. Experimental Gerontology37: 477–489. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
20. Dutta C, Hadley EC, Lexell J (1997) Sarcopenia and physical performance in old age: overview. Muscle Nerve5: S5–S9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. Hirsch CH, Fried LP, Harris T, Fitzpatrick A, Enright P, et al. (1997) Correlates of performance based measures of muscle function in the elderly: The cardiovascular health study. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences52A(4): M192–M200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
22. Lexell J (1995) Human aging, muscle mass and fiber type composition. Journal of Gerontology50A (Special Issue): 11–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
23. Lexell J (1997) Evidence for nervous system degeneration with advancing age. The Journal of Nutrition127: 1011S–1013S. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
24. Narici MV, Maffulli N (2010) Sarcopenia: characteristics, mechanisms and functional significance. British Medical Bulletin95: 139–159. 10.1093/bmb/ldq008 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
25. Metter EJ, Schrager M, Ferrucci L, Talbot LA (2005) Evaluation of movement speed and reaction time as predictors of all-cause mortality in men. Journal of Gerontology: Biological Sciences60A(7): 840–846. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Kallman DA, Plato CC, Tobin JD (1990) The role of muscle strength loss in the age-related decline in grip strength: cross-sectional and longitudinal perspectives. Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences45: M82–M88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Kauranen K, Vanharanta H (1996) Influences of aging, gender and handedness on Motor Performance of upper and lower extremities. Perceptual and Motor skills82: 515–525. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
28. Vianna LC, Oliviera RB, Araújo CGS (2007) Age-related decline in handgrip strength differs according to gender. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research21(4): 1310–1314. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
29. Mathiowetz V, Kashman N, Volland G, Weber K, Dowe M, et al. (1985) Grip and Pinch strength: normative data for adults. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation66: 69–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
30. Rantanen T, Masaki K, Foley D, Izmirlian G, White L, et al. (1998) Grip strength changes over 27yr in Japanese-American men. Journal of Applied Physiology85: 2047–2053. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
31. Metter EJ, Conwit R, Metter B, Pacheco T, Tobin J (1998) The relationship of peripheral motor nerve conduction velocity to age-associated loss of grip strength. Aging (Milano)10: 471–478. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
32. Dorfman LJ, Bosley TM (1979) Age related changes in peripheral and central nerve conduction in man. Neurology16: 38–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
33. Kurokawa K, Mimori Y, Tanaka E, Kohriyama T, Nakamura S (1999) Age-Related Change in Peripheral Nerve Conduction: Compound Muscle Action Potential Duration and Dispersion. Gerontology45: 168–173. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
34. Mackenzie RA, Phillips LH (1981) Changes in peripheral and central nerve conduction with aging. Clinical Experimental Neurology18: 109–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
35. Kaplan FS, Nixon JE, Reitz M, Rindfleish L, Tucker J (1985) Age-related changes in proprioception and sensation of joint position. Acta Orthopaedica Scandinavica56(1): 72–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
36. Payne AM, Delbono O (2004) Neurogenesis of excitation-contraction uncoupling in aging skeletal muscle. Exercise and Sport Science Review32: 36–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) 'Mini-mental state'. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research12(3): 189–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
38. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia9(1): 97–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
39. Schaubert KL, Bohannon RW (2005) Reliability and validity of three strength measures obtained from community-dwelling elderly persons. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research19: 717–720. 10.1016/j.bios.2015.01.017 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
40. Lafayette Instruments (2013) https://www.lafayettelifesciences.com/
41. Schoppe KJ (1974) Das, MLS-Gerät. Ein neuer Testapparat zur Messung feinmotorischer Leistungen. Diagnostica20: 43–47. [Google Scholar]
42. Fleishman EA (1972) Structure and measurement of psychomotor abilities In: Singer RN, editor. The psychomotor domain. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger; pp. 78–196. [Google Scholar]
43. Raczek J, Waskiewicz Z, Juras G (1997) Raum-, zeitliche- und dynamische Aspekte koordinativ-motorischer Leistungsvoraussetzungen—ein Objektivierungsversuch der Diagnostik In: Hirtz P, Nuske F, editors. Bewegungskoordination und sportliche Leistung integrativ betrachten. Schriften der Deutschen Vereinigung für Sportwissenschaft. Czwalina Verlag, Hamburg, 87, pp. 293–300. [Google Scholar]
44. Raczek J, Mynarski W, Ljach W (1998) Theoretical and empirical basis for developing and diagnosing of co-ordination motor abilities. Acad. of Ph. Ed., Katowice: (in Polish, English summary). [Google Scholar]
45. Raczek J, Juras G, Waśkiewicz Z (2001) The diagnosis of motor coordination. Journal of Human Kinetics6: 113–125. [Google Scholar]
46. Fleishman EA (1953) Testing for psychomotor abilities by means of apparatus tests. Psychological Bulletin50: S241–S262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
47. Cohen J, Cohen P (1983) Applied multiple regression/correlation analyses for the behavioural sciences (2nd edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
48. Aitken LS & West SG (1991) Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
49. Bassey EJ, Harries UJ (1993) Normal values for hand grip strength in 920 men and women aged over 65 years, and longitudinal changes over 4 years in 620 survivors. Clinical Science84: 331–337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
50. Bowden JJ, McNulty PA (2013) The magnitude and rate of reduction in strength, dexterity and sensation in the human hand very with ageing. Experimental Gerontology48: 756–765. 10.1016/j.exger.2013.03.011 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
51. Desrosiers J, Hebert R, Bravo G, Rochette A (1998) Comparison of Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Designs in the Study of Aging of Upper Extremity Performance. Journal of Geronotology: Biological Sciences53A: B362–B368. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
52. Desrosiers J, Hébert R, Bravo G, Rochette A (1999) Age-related changes in upper extremity performance of elderly people: a longitudinal study. Experimental Gerontology, 34: 393–405. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Manual Dexterity Activities For Adults
53. Hinson M, Gench B (1989) The curvilinear relationship of grip strength to age. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research9: 53–60. [Google Scholar]
54. Larsson L, Grimby G, Karlsson J (1979) Muscle strength and speed of movement in relation to age and muscle morphology. Journal Applied Physiology46(3): 451–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
55. Doherty TJ, Brown WF (1997) Age-related changes in the twitch contractile properties of human thenar motor units. Journal of Applied Physiology82: 93–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
56. Doherty TJ, Vandervoort AA, Taylor AW, Brown WF (1993) Effects of motor unit losses on strength in older men and women. Journal of Applied Physiology74: 868–874. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
57. Bouche P, Cattelin F, Saint-Jean O, Léger JM, Queslati S, et al. (1993) Clinical and electrophysiological study of the peripheral nervous system in the elderly. Journal of Neurology240: 263–268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
58. Norris AH, Shock NW, Wagman IH (1953) Age changes in the maximum conduction velocity of motor fibers of human ulnar nerves. Journal of Applied Physiology5: 589–593. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
59. Macaluso A, Nimmo MA, Foster JE, Cockburn M, McMillan NC, et al. (2002) Contractile muscle volume and agonist-antagonist coactivation account for differences in torque between young and older women. Muscle Nerve25: 858–863. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
60. Desrosiers J, Bravo G, Hébert R, Dutil É (1994) Validation of the Box and Block Test as a measure of dexterity of elderly people: reliability, validity, and norms studies. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation75(7): 751–755. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
61. Desrosiers J, Hébert R, Bravo G, Dutil É (1995) Upper extremity performance test for the elderly (TEMPA): Normative data and correlates with sensorimotor parameters. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation76: 1125–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
62. Michimata A, Kondo T, Suzukamo Y, Chiba M (2008) The Manual Function test: Norms for 20-to 90-year-olds and effects of age, gender, and hand dominance on Dexterity. The Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine214: 257–267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
63. Mitrushina MN (2005) Handbook of Normative Data for Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press: New York: [Google Scholar]
64. Cole K (2006) Age-related directional bias of fingertip force. Experimental Brain Research175: 285–291. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
65. Galganski ME, Fuglevand AJ, Enoka RM (1993) Reduced control of motor output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during sub maximal contractions. Journal of Neurophysiology69(6): 2108–2115. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
66. Graves AE, Kornatz KW, Enoka RM (2000) Older adults use a unique strategy to lift inertial loads with the elbow flexor muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology83(4): 2030–2039. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
67. McDonagh MJ, White MJ, Davies CT (1984) Different effects of ageing on the mechanical properties of human arm and leg muscles. Gerontology30: 49–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
68. Voelcker-Rehage C, Alberts JL (2005) Age-related changes in grasping force modulation. Experimental Brain Research166: 61–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
69. Macaluso A, DeVito G (2004) Muscle strength, power and adaptations to resistance training in older people. European Journal of Applied Physiology91: 450–472. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
70. Häkkinen K, Kallinen M, Izquierdo M, Jokelainen K, Lassila H, et al. (1998) Changes in agonist-antagonist EMG, muscle CSA, and force during strength training in middle-aged and older people. Journal of Applied Physiology84(4): 1341–1349. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
71. Hortobágyi T, Devita P (2006) Mechanisms responsible for the age associated increase in coactivation of antagonist muscles. Exercise Sport Science Reviews34(1) 29–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
72. Barry BK, Pascoe MA, Jesunathadas M, Enoka RM (2007) Rate coding is compressed but variability is unaltered for motor units in a hand muscle of old adults. Journal of Neurophysiology97(5): 3206–3218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
73. Elble RJ, Higgins C, Leffler K, Hughes L (1994) Factors influencing the Amplitude and Frequency of Essential Tremor. Movement Disorders9(6): 589–596. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
74. Elble RJ (1995) The role of aging in the clinical expression of essential tremor. Experimental Gerontology30: 337–347. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
75. Elble RJ (2000) Essential tremor frequency decreases with time. Neurology55: 1547–1551. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
76. Trombly CA (1989) Occupational therapy for physical dysfunction (3rd edition). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
77. Zoltan B, Pedretti LW (1990) Evaluation of muscle tone and coordination In: Pedretti LW, Zoltan B, editors. Occupational therapy: practice skills for physical dysfunction (3rd edition). St-Louis: CV Mosby. [Google Scholar]
78. Farber SD (1991) Assessing neuromotor performance enablers In: Christiansen C, Baum C, editors. Occupational therapy: overcoming human performance deficits. Thorofore, NJ: Slack. [Google Scholar]
79. Cole KJ (1991) Grasp force control in older adults. Journal of Motor Behaviour23: 251–258. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
80. Kinoshita H, Francis PR (1996) A comparison of prehension force control in young and elderly individuals. European Journal of Applied Physiology74: 450–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
81. Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology47: 381–391. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
82. de C Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM (2002) Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance. Journal of Neurophysiology87: 2434–2440. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
83. Kim S, Carlton LG, Liu YT, Newell KM (1999) Impulse and movement space-time variability. Journal of Motor Behaviour31: 341–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
84. van Galen GP, van Huygevoort M (2000) Error, stress and the role of neuromotor noise in space oriented behaviour. Biological Psychology51: 151–171. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
85. Carlton LG, Newell KM (1993) Force variability and characteristics of force production In: Newell KM, Cordo PM, editors. Force Variability. Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL: pp. 128–135. [Google Scholar]
86. Christou EA, Carlton LG (2002) Age and contraction type influence motor output variability in rapid discrete tasks. Journal of Applied Physiology92: 489–498. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
87. Christou EA, Carlton LG (2001) Old adults exhibit greater motor output variability than young adults only during rapid discrete isometric contractions. Journal Gerontology56: B524–B532. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
88. Christou EA, Tracy BL, Enoka RM (2002) Steadiness of lengthening contractions In: Latash M, editor. Progress in Motor Control II. Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL: pp. 195–207. [Google Scholar]
89. Cordo P, Carlton L, Bevan L, Carlton M, Kerr GK (1994) Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. Journal of Neurophysiology71: 1848–1861. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
90. Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature394: 780–784. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
91. Bilodeau M, Keen DA, Sweeney PJ, Shields RW, Enoka RM (2000) Strength training can improve steadiness in persons with essential tremor. Muscle Nerve23: 771–778. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
92. Keen DA, Yue GH, Enoka RM (1994) Training-related enhancement in the control of motor output in elderly humans. Journal of Applied Physiology77: 2648–2658. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
93. Laidlaw DH, Kornatz KW, Keen DA, Suzuki S, Enoka RM (1999) Strength training improves the steadiness of slow lengthening contractions performed by old adults. Journal of Applied Physiology87: 1786–1795. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
94. Tracy BL, Kern DS, Mehoudar PD, Sehnert S, Enoka RM (2001) Strength training does not improve the steadiness of muscle contractions in the knee extensors of older adults. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise33: S25410.1016/j.bbalip.2015.01.004 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
95. Krampe R, Ericsson K (1996). Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General125: 331–359. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
96. Krampe R (2002) Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews26: 769–776. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of Public Library of Science
(Redirected from Manual dexterity)
Writing is a fine motor skill as it requires subtle motions of the hand and fingers.

Fine motor skill (or dexterity) is the coordination of small muscles, in movements—usually involving the synchronisation of hands and fingers—with the eyes. The complex levels of manual dexterity that humans exhibit can be attributed to and demonstrated in tasks controlled by the nervous system. Fine motor skills aid in the growth of intelligence and develop continuously throughout the stages of human development.

  • 2Developmental stages

Types of motor skills[edit]

Motor skills are movements and actions of the bone structures.[1] Typically, they are categorised into two groups: gross motor skills and fine motor skills. Gross motor skills are involved in movement and coordination of the arms, legs, and other large body parts. They involve actions such as running, crawling and swimming. Fine motor skills are involved in smaller movements that occur in the wrists, hands, fingers, feet and toes. They involve smaller actions such as picking up objects between the thumb and finger, writing carefully, and even blinking. These two motor skills work together to provide coordination.

Developmental stages[edit]

Through each developmental stage of a child’s life and throughout our lifetime motor skills gradually develop. They are first seen during a child’s development stages: infancy, toddler-hood, preschool and school age. 'Basic” fine motor skills gradually develop and are typically mastered between the ages of 6-12 in children. These skills will keep developing with age, practice and the increased use of muscles while playing sports, playing an instrument, using the computer, and writing. If deemed necessary, occupational therapy can help improve overall fine motor skills.[2]

Infancy[edit]

The first motor skills, beginning from birth, are initially characterised by involuntary reflexes.[3] The most notable involuntary reflex is the Darwinian reflex, a primitive reflex displayed in various newborn primates species. These involuntary muscle movements are temporary and often disappear after the first two months. After eight weeks, the infant will begin to voluntarily use their fingers to touch. However, their ability to grab objects is still undeveloped at this point.

Infant displaying the palmar grasp

At two to five months the infant will begin to develop hand-eye coordination, and they will start reaching for and grasping objects. In this way, they improve their overall grasping skills.

In 1952, Piaget found that even before infants are able to reach for and successfully grasp objects they see, they demonstrate competent hand-mouth coordination. A study was done by Philippe Rochat at Emory University in 1992, to test the relation between progress in the control of posture and the developmental transition from two handed to one handed engagement in reaching. It was found that the object reached for needed to be controlled. The precision of the reach is potentially maximized when placed centrally. It was also found that the posture needed to be controlled because infants that were not able to sit on their own used bimanual reaches in all postural positions except sitting upright, where they would reach one-handed. As a result, their grasping phases will not have been maximized because of the decrease in body control. On the other hand, if the infant does not have body control, it would be hard for them to get a hold of an object because their reach will be limited. As a result, the infant will just keep falling, stopping them from reaching an object because of no body control. When 'nonsitting' infants reached bimanually, while seated upright, they often ended up falling forward which prevented them from reaching toward the target. Regardless of their ability or lack of ability to control self-sitting, infants are able to adjust their two handed engagement in relation to the arrangement of the objects being reached for. Analysis of hand-to-hand distance during reaching indicates that in the prone and supine posture, non-sitting infants moved their hands simultaneously towards the midline of their bodies as they reached which is not observed by stable sitting infants in any position. Non-sitter infants, although showing strong tendencies toward bimanual reaching, tend to reach with one hand only, when placed in the seated posture. Sitter infants show a majority of differentiated reaches in all posture conditions.

A study conducted by Esther Thelen on postural control during infancy used the dynamic systems approach to observe motor development. The findings suggest that early reaching is constrained by head and shoulder instability. The relationship between posture and reaching cannot be disentangled. Thus, head control and body stability are necessary for the emergence of grasping.

The next developmental milestone is between seven and twelve months, when a series of fine motor skills begins to develop. These include, but are not limited to, increase in grip, enhancement of vision, pointing with the index finger, smoothly transferring objects from one hand to the other, as well as using the pincer grip (with the thumb and index fingers) to pick up tiny objects with precision. A lot of factors change in grasping when the infant becomes seven months. The infant will have better chance of grasping due to the fact that the infant can sit up on their own. Therefore, the infant will not fall over. The infant grasping also changes. The infant starts to hold objects more properly when age increases[4]

Toddler-hood[edit]

Writing abilities are a major fine motor skill.

By the time a child is one year old, their fine motor skills have developed to allow the manipulation of objects with greater intent.

As children manipulate objects with purpose, they gain experience identifying objects based on their shape, size, and weight. By engaging in hands-on play the child learns that some objects are heavy, requiring more force to move them; that some are small, easily slipping through the fingers; and that other objects come apart and can possibly be put back together again. This type of play is essential for the development of not only the child's fine motor skills, but also for learning how the world works.[5]

It is during this stage in the development of fine motor skills that a toddler will show hand dominance.

Preschool[edit]

Children typically attend preschool between the ages of 2 and 5. At this time, the child is capable of grasping objects using the static tripod grasp, which is the combined use of the index, thumb, and middle finger. A preschooler's motor skills are moderate, allowing the child to cut shapes out of paper, draw or trace over vertical lines with crayons, button their clothes, and pick up objects. A preferred hand dominates the majority of their activities. They also develop sensory awareness and interpret their environment by using their senses and coordinate movements based on that.[6]

After the static tripod grasp, the next form is the dynamic tripod grasp. These are shown in a series through Schneck and Henderson's Grip Form chart. Based on the accuracy and form of hold the child will be ranked either from 1-10 or 1-5 of how well they are able to complete the dynamic tripod grasp while properly writing. In conjunction with accuracy and precision the child will be able to properly position a writing utensil in terms of implement diameter as well as form and grip strength. Proper handwriting and drawing fall deeper into a category of graphomotor skills. [7]

Manual Dexterity Tasks

The National Center of Teaching and Learning illustrates the abilities that preschoolers should have improved through their fine motor skills in several domains. Children use their motor skills by sorting and manipulating geometric shapes, making patterns, and using measurement tools to build their math skills. By using writing tools and reading books, they build their language and literacy. Arts and crafts activities like cutting and gluing paper, finger painting, and dressing up develops their creativity. Parents can support this development by intervening when the child does not perform the fine motor activity correctly, making use of several senses in a learning activity, and offer activities that the child will be successful with.[6]

Developmental disabilities may render a child incapable of performing certain motor activities, such as drawing or building blocks.[8] Fine motor skills acquired during this stage aids in the later advancement and understanding of subjects such as science and reading.[9] A study by the American Journal of Occupational Therapy, which included twenty-six preschoolers who had received occupational therapy on a weekly basis, showed overall advancements in their fine motor skill area. The results showed a link between in-hand manipulation, eye-hand coordination, and grasping strength with the child's motor skills, self-care and social function. In addition, these children were shown to have better mobility and self-sustainment.[2]

School age[edit]

During the ages between 5 and 7 the fine motor skills will have developed to a much higher degree, and are now being refined. As the child interacts with objects the movements of the elbows and shoulders should be less apparent, as should the movements of wrist and fingers. From the ages of 3–5 years old, girls advance their fine motor skills more than boys. Girls develop physically at an earlier age than boys; this is what allows them to advance their motor skills at a faster rate during prepubescent ages. Boys advance in gross motor skills later on at around age 5 and up. Girls are more advanced in balance and motor dexterity.[citation needed]

Children should be able to make precise cuts with scissors, for example, cutting out squares and holding them in a more common and mature manner. The child's movements should become fluid as the arms and hands become more in sync with each other. The child should also be able to write more precisely on lines, and print letters and numbers with greater clarity. In terms of motor development and athletic performance, pediatric boys[clarification needed] tend to be much more physically active than pediatric girls by nature and have a harder time staying still for long periods of time. This is due to the early development of motor skills that occurs in boys faster than it does in girls. During the first 2–3 years of elementary school, gross motor skills are similar among girls and boys with basic skills such as being able to run, jump, and toss a ball. However, boys start to develop more gross motor skills that give them an advantage in activities where girls may still be working on the basics. Boys' high energy and choice to be a part of large groups comes from their gross motor skills being developed. In general, pediatric girls tend to fall behind pediatric boys in terms of advancement of gross motor skills toward the end of elementary school.[10]https://courses.lumenlearning.com/educationalpsychology/chapter/gender-differences-in-the-classroom/

Common problems[edit]

Fine motor skills can become impaired due to injury, illness, stroke, congenital deformities, cerebral palsy, or developmental disabilities. Problems with the brain, spinal cord, peripheral nerves, muscles, or joints can also have an effect on fine motor skills, and can decrease control. If an infant or child up to age five is not developing their fine motor skills, they will show signs of difficulty controlling coordinated body movements with the hands, fingers, and face. In young children, the delay in the ability to sit up or learn to walk can be an early sign that there will be issues with fine motor skills. Children may also show signs of difficulty with tasks such as cutting with scissors, drawing lines, folding clothes, holding a pencil and writing, and zipping a zipper. These are tasks that involve fine motor skills, and if a child has difficulty with these they might have poor hand eye coordination and could need therapy to improve their skills.

Assessment[edit]

Manual Dexterity Activities For Adults For Adults

Fine motor skills can be assessed with standardized and non-standardized tests in children and adults. Fine-motor assessments can include force matching tasks. Humans exhibit a high degree of accuracy in force matching tasks where an individual is instructed to match a reference force applied to a finger with the same or different finger.[11] Humans also exhibit a high degree of accuracy during grip force matching tasks.[12] These aspects of manual dexterity are apparent in the ability of humans to effectively use tools, and perform challenging manipulation tasks such as handling unstable objects.[13] Other assessments include but are not limited to PDMS 'The Peabody Developmental Scales'.[14] PDMS is an evaluation done for children from birth till the age seven that examines the child's ability to grasp a variety of objects, the development of eye-hand coordination, and the child's overall finger dexterity.[14] Similar to PDMS, Visual-motor integration assessment, VMI-R, is an assessment that examines the visual motor integration system which demonstrates and points out possible learning disabilities that are often related to delays in visual perception and fine-motor skills such as poor hand-eye coordination.[15] Because additionally advancements in mathematics and language skills are directly corollated to the development of the fine motor system, it is essential that children acquire the fine motor skills that are needed to interact with the environment at an early stage.[16] Examples of tests include:

References[edit]

  1. ^'Fine motor control: MedlinePlus Medical Encyclopedia'. medlineplus.gov. Retrieved 2019-05-06.
  2. ^ ab'Fine Motor Outcomes in Preschool Children Who Receive Occupational Therapy Services'. Fine Motor Outcomes in Preschool Children Who Receive Occupational Therapy Services. Retrieved 26 October 2014.
  3. ^Wells, Ken R. 'Fine Motor Skills.' The Gale Encyclopedia of Children's Health: Infancy through Adolescence. Ed. Kristine Krapp and Jeffrey Wilson. Vol. 2. Detroit: Gale, 2006. 756-760. Gale Virtual Reference Library. Web. 28 Oct. 2014.
  4. ^'Fine Motor Skills & Activities for Infants & Toddlers'. Early Intervention Support.
  5. ^'Play Activities to Encourage Motor Development in Child Care'. Extension.org. Retrieved 26 November 2014.
  6. ^ ab'Domain 8: Physical Health & Development'. Domain 8: Physical Health & Development. Retrieved 9 December 2014.
  7. ^Burton, Allen. 'Grip Form and Graphomotor Control in Preschool Children'. AJOT. Retrieved 1 March 2018.
  8. ^Grissmer, David, et al. 'Fine Motor Skills And Early Comprehension Of The World: Two New School Readiness Indicators.' Developmental Psychology 46.5 (2010): 1008-1017. PsycARTICLES.
  9. ^'Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators'. APA PsycNET. Retrieved 26 October 2014.
  10. ^'School Aged Developmental Milestones'.
  11. ^Park WH, Leonard CT, Li S (August 2008). 'Finger force perception during ipsilateral and contralateral force matching tasks'. Exp Brain Res. 189 (3): 301–10. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1424-7. PMC2889908. PMID18488212.
  12. ^Harrison LM, Mayston MJ, Johansson RS (September 2000). 'Reactive control of precision grip does not depend on fast transcortical reflex pathways in X-linked Kallmann subjects'. J. Physiol. 527 Pt 3 (3): 641–52. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00641.x. PMC2270096. PMID10990548.
  13. ^Venkadesan, M; Guckenheimer, John; Valero-Cuevas, Francisco J. (2007). 'Manipulating the edge of instability'. Journal of Biomechanics. 40 (8): 1653–61. doi:10.1016/j.jbiomech.2007.01.022. PMC2666355. PMID17400231.
  14. ^ abMaddox, T. (2007). Peabody developmental motor scales. In Encyclopedia of special education: A reference for the education of children, adolescents, and adults with disabilities and other exceptional individuals.
  15. ^Fuchs, D., Tenorio, Y., Bromley, M., and Fuchs, L. (2007). Visual-motor integration. In Encyclopedia of special education: A reference for the education of children, adolescents, and adults with disabilities and other exceptional individuals.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^Grissmer, David (2010). 'Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators'. Developmental Psychology. 46 (5): 1008–17. doi:10.1037/a0020104. PMID20822219.

External links[edit]

Dexterity Exercises For Adults

Retrieved from 'https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Fine_motor_skill&oldid=915911597'